Appreciating Neurodiversity: Learning From Synesthesia

This blog post originally appeared in the Emory Neuroethics blog (theneuroethicsblog.com/2013/04/appreciating-neurodiversity-learning.html) and the Neuroethics Women Leaders blog (neuroethicswomenleaders.com/2013/04/26/appreciating-neurodiversity-learning-from-synesthesia)
 
Who are synesthetes?

[Describing the experiences of subject MMo] Eights are yellow, for example, a square feels like mashed potatoes, and the name Steve is somehow like poached eggs. (Cytowic p. 26)

“…I [asked the vendor] what kind of ice cream she had.  ‘Fruit ice cream,’ she said.  But she answered in such a tone that a whole pile of coals, of black cinders, came bursting out of her mouth, and I couldn’t bring myself to buy any ice cream after she’d answered that way…” (Record of patient “S”, Luria p. 82)

Colored alphabet via Wikipedia Commons

The unexpected sensory pairings described above are the experiences of a minority of people, perhaps 4% of the population (Simner et al., 2006), known as synesthetes. Synesthesia is a perceptual phenomenon in which unusual linkages occur between sensory modalities – sounds may evoke colors, tastes may evoke shapes, or numbers may evoke spatial patterns – in all, over 40 unique pairings have been documented.  Synesthesia creates problems – it defies normal categories of clinical pathology, and yet is clearly different from what we might call “normal” or neurotypical perception. But examining the phenomenon can help us gain a greater understanding of how synesthetes perceive the world, how others perceive them, and in what ways neuroscience can help us better understand unusual neurological phenotypes – what we might call neurodiversity.

A brief history of synesthesia

The earliest known documentation of synesthesia comes from an Austrian physician who provided a self-description of his own “colored ideas” (Sachs 1812, Jewanski et al., 2009).  Sir Francis Galton provided some of the first descriptions of individuals who conceive of numbers in specific, idiosyncratic spatial forms (Galton 1880, 1881). Not long after, physician and poet Victor Ségalen reported an even wider variety of linkages, drawn from multiple first-person accounts: The poet Arthur Rimbaud experiences the letter A as black; a math prodigy describes Monday as light chestnut brown; a Jesuit priest describes his sound to color synesthesia as an “ocular harpsichord” (Ségalen 1902).

Cytowic RE and Eagleman DM,
Wednesday is Indigo Blue:
Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia

The unusual nature of these reports combined with inherent difficulty of verifying personal accounts has frequently lead researchers to conclude that synesthetes are either confabulating, schizophrenic, or using hallucinogens (Day 2002). As a result, many synesthestes feel their personal sensoria render them abnormal, alien – different from “normals” or non-synesthetes. Some who learned to suppress reports of their sensory experiences later borrow language from gay rights movement – “closet synesthetes” will “come out” to family and friends (Day 2002). Modern neuroscientific and psychological methodologies have established the synesthesia as empirically measurable and therefore valid discrete neurological “condition”. Medicalizing synesthesia in this way can both validate synesthetes’ experiences, but also raises some ethical problems.  Scientific researchers and clinicians are often ill-equipped to comment on diversity, but when neuroscientists and neurologists make scientific claims about “normal” and “non-normal” brains, they do, perhaps unintentionally, make identity claims about the owners of those brains. Referring to synesthesia as a “neurological condition” may marginalize and de-legitimize synesthetic experience in the eyes of some, even as it validates it as a “real” experience.  Next, we’ll the research of clinicians who have examined the cognitive impacts of synesthetic experience, without pathologizing the condition, in an attempt to understand the variation between synesthetes and neurotypicals.

Synesthesia and the mechanisms of metaphor

Metaphors like “sharp cheese” and “bitter cold” are understood by most English speakers, despite the seemingly arbitrary nature of the cross-modal pairings. The appreciation of metaphor by synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike suggests a tie between synesthesia and metaphor (Ramachandran and Hubbard, 2001a).  Other arbitrary pairings are appreciated by synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike – in one experiment, a soft curved shape and a sharp jagged shape are presented, and subjects are asked to guess which shape is named Kiki, and which Bouba.  Most pick Bouba for the curved shape, and Kiki for the jagged shape (Ramachandran and Hubbard 2001b; Köhler 1947). The cortical substrate responsible for the phenomenon is the angular gyrus (Ramachandran and Hubbard 2005), an area already implicated some number-spatial forms of synesthesia from a clinical case – a synesthete who lost his synesthetic number forms after a bullet tore through the same brain region (Spalding and Zangwill 1950).

Psychologist A. R. Luria’s patient S was a synesthete with multiple forms of intense synesthethias; so intense, they were impossible for him to ignore. Interestingly, he had great difficulty with metaphor. When asked to explain the expression “to weigh one’s words”, S responds:

“When I hear the word weigh, I see a large scale […] where they put bread on one side and a weight on the other.  The arrow shifts to one side, then stops in the middle… But what do you have here – to weigh one’s words!” [Luria p. 119]

But is S representative of all synesthetes?  Scores of talented synesthetic artists, writers, and musicians suggest otherwise (examples include Kandinsky, Nabokov, Itzhak Perlman, and Stevie Wonder).  The fact that synesthesia appears to play an important role in the art produced contradicts the notion of a trade-off – perhaps synesthetic perceptions support facility with metaphor, and other forms of artistic expression.

Kandinsky’s Reiter, via Wikipedia Commons

The question remains – why do the arbitrary associations of metaphor appeal to so many? If linkages are arbitrary, shouldn’t they be idiosyncratic, as well? Surprisingly, recent work suggests that arbitrary cross-modal pairings in synesthetes may not be as arbitrary as we thought – among synesthetes who experience letter-color pairings, Baron-Cohen and colleagues (1993) discovered subtle, non-random patterns – U is often yellow to light brown, I and O are often white, and X is often black. Even among synesthetes, where idiosyncrasy is the rule – commonalities emerge.

Learning from Neurodiversity and the role of the neuroscientist

If non-synesthetes also appreciate and employ metaphor, is it possible that there are other features of experience that synesthetes and non-synesthetes share? Do these neurophenotypes overlap – and how?

JM: “…the name Paul is such an ugly color, its [sic] gray and ugly. I told her ‘anything but Paul’. And she couldn’t understand why and I said ‘it is such an ugly color, that name Paul.’ […] The name probably isn’t that bad, but in my mind it’s very awful. And that influences how I feel about people”. [Cytowic, p. 45]

Although synesthetic experiences blur the usually rigid boundaries of perceptual modalities and conceptual categories in ways that seem arbitrary, synesthesia offer a new framework for understanding how non-synesthete, or “normal” perceptual and conceptual processing are constituted.  I propose that although different from normal perception, synesthetic perceptions may be the manifestation of an extreme end of a continuum of human variation, in which implicit cross-sensory linkages “break through” into explicit perception. The ability of synesthetic artists of different kinds to reach broad audiences despite the idiosyncratic and rather arbitrary nature of their associations may belie some common, unconscious, less arbitrary associations that may resonate with neurotypicals. A final anecdote from a synesthete watching a figure skating routine seems to suggest that synesthetes, in their “abnormality”, see that which the non-synesthete cannot see but somehow also knows (emphasis mine):

“A young woman […] skated to a piece of music that was green and full of squares.  Her costume was totally black; her routine started out with curving motions.  It became uncomfortable to me to watch such a mismatch, so I just looked at my hands in my lap.  She placed last in her category at the end of the evening’s judging.” [Record of subject MLL; Cytowic p. 38]

Neuroscientists have an ethical obligation to take heed of these synesthete self-reports – not only do they underscore the rich neurodiversity that exists among us, but they also may lead to new, exciting areas for research. If, as I have suggested, cross-sensory pairings exist in all of us, studying the neural substrates that support synesthesia will illuminate common mechanisms of perception in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike. Neuroscientists should remember that the boundaries between normal and abnormal are not always bright lines – and in the case of synesthesia, understanding synesthetes and neurotypicals as existing on a continuum of human neurodiversity can help us gain new insight into how perception operates in all of us.

References

Baron-Cohen S, Harrison J, Goldstein LH, Wyke M. 1993. Coloured speech perception: Is synaesthesia what happens when modularity breaks down? Perception 22: 419-426.

Cytowic RE. 2002. Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses. MIT Press. 394 pp.

Day S. 2005. Some demographic and socio-cultural aspects of synesthesia. In Robertson LC, Sagiv N, eds. Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. 266 pp.

Galton Sir F. 1881. Visualized numerals. Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 10: 85-102.

Galton Sir F. 1880.Visualised Numerals. Nature. 252-256.

Jewanski J; Day SA, Ward J. 2009. A colorful albino: The first documented case of synaesthesia, by Georg
Tobias Ludwig Sachs in 1812. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 18: 293-303.

Köhler W. 1947.Gestalt Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Liveright.

Luria AR. 1968. The Mind of the Mnemonist: A Little Book About a Vast Memory. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

Nabokov V. ed. 1999. Speak, Memory.  New York: Everyman’s Library. 344 pp.

Ramachandran VS, Hubbard A. 2001a. Psychological investigations into the neural basis of synesthesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 268: 979-983.

Ramachandran VS, Hubbard A. 2001b. The Emergence of the Human Mind: Some Clues from Synesthesia. In Robertson LC, Sagiv N, eds. Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. 266 pp.

Sachs GTL. 1812. Historiae naturalis duorum leucaetiopum. Auctoris ipsius et sororis eius. Erlangen [Online].

Ségalen, V. 1902. Les Synesthésies et l’Ecole Symboliste. Montpellier.

Simner J, Mulvenna C, Sagiv N, Tsakanikos E, Witherby SA, Fraser C, Scott K, Ward J. 2006.
Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception 35:1024-1033.

Spalding JMK, Zangwill O. 1950. Disturbance of number-form in a case of brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 12: 24-29.

Advertisement

5 thoughts on “Appreciating Neurodiversity: Learning From Synesthesia

  1. I link the calendar, i.e. Days, months, years and centuries with colour. The weeks and years are circular while the centuries are parallel and the years run bottom to top.

  2. Very interesting article vith several novel points (my keyboard at present lacks several letters, sorry)
    I’m alvays so ambivalent about sharing particulars. Even though, for example, the latter ‘A’ has been for me since birth (or as long as I remember cardinal Red, and could never be othervise, hearing another synesthete describe it as a different colour gives me a sad and queasy feeling, as if speaking to a disguised alien.
    I do use the musical ‘crossover’, in conjunction vith perfect pitch simultaneously: they are basically the same experiential bath, so to speak.
    I do often vonder hov I got this curse/blessing. Articles like this one help to at least ‘dignify’ it, and I no longer think of it as ‘bedvetting’ or some such shameful oddness. JS/Tel Aviv

    • You might be interested in Richard Cytowic’s book on Synesthesia (http://www.amazon.com/Synesthesia-Union-Senses-Second-Edition/dp/0262032961) where he describes what you’re talking about – some synesthetes experience visceral negative feelings at “mismatches” as you describe above.

      It’s really interesting that you have perfect pitch that accompanies your synesthetic experience (if I’m understanding you correctly). Some studies have indicated that as infants, we are capable of something like “perfect pitch”, but that most of us lose this ability during development. Others have suggested that synesthetic associations may be more prevalent in children than adults – perhaps most of us have some sensory crosstalk but usually lose it later in life.

      For what it’s worth, I also experience synesthesia, in my case for numbers, weeks, months, and years. They are linked to spatial representations (similar to the second figure, but with no color associations). I also have strong photographic memories for word spellings, which I wonder, like your perfect pitch, might be another, related quirk of neurological development particular to synesthetes.

      Thanks for your comment, and glad you enjoyed the post!

  3. My associate, Steven Summer, of the Blake’s 7 obsession story, is synesthesiatic, or synesthete. Some of the B7 obsession story is featured on wordpress.

  4. Thank you so much for your reply and apologies on the delayed response. Time zones, etc. Yes, I’d seen Cytowic cited as e reference, might be time to read the book. I envy your numbers, dates, etc residing in and organized ‘frame’. One of the curious parts is trying to understand hov and vhen these constructions started. I do recall in first grade colouring in the number eight as yellov as our cheap crayolas alloved, But it’s almost a chicken vs egg question
    Speaking from the heart a second, I grev up knovin I vas ‘different’ in many vays, but egotistically usually assumed that most of my cohorts vere simply ‘retarded’. In the end, they rule the vorld and I’m simply odd. Oh vell.
    Once again, thanks for your time and attention.

Your thoughts?

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s